uncovering the social bases of urban differentiation. Factor analysis in urban studies comes from a long tradition grounded in sociology and geography. In this context, it is used as a method to study the underlying social structure it is not viewed as a technique for feature engineering in this work. Rather, it is an attempt to understand whether social systems differ in similar ways across different spatial contexts (e.g. comparing factorial ecologies), or testing whether a theoretical social construct is associated with an array of outcomes (ecometrics & structural equation modeling)
Following the introduction of BCZ, empirical models of spatial structure went largely undeveloped for a significant period. In the 1950s, however, social shifts yet again brought emphasis on the significance of place. During the postwar period of suburbanization, white flight, and social unrest, there was a significant push to understand the nature of “community;” the Chicago School focused strongly on how urban spatial segmentation leads to social behaviors like territorialism and consciously created “ideological communities” (Hawley, 1950; Hunter, 1975; Suttles, 1972).
To understand how these communities were created, scholars turned to newly developed statistical methods to identify the essential elements of “urbanism” that structure modern life and the field of social area analysis was born. This turn is often viewed as the beginning of an age of urban empiricism, but it is important to emphasize that a critical component of Chicago School analysis is empirical work that is grounded firmly in social theory (Sampson2002?). As researchers adopted new statistical techniques, therefore, they attempted to operationalize “the complicated phenomena of urbanism,” described by (Wirth1938?) as “a system of social organization involving a characteristic social structure, a series of social institutions, and a typical pattern of social relations”.
Seeking to formalize and operationalize the ideas of human ecology, neighborhoods, and social areas, researchers set out to define neighborhoods as a set of spatially structured social interactions. A neighborhood or “natural area” could then be identified as meeting the following criteria:
- a geographic area physically distinguishable from other adjacent areas;
- a population with unique social, demographic, or ethnic composition;
- a social system with rules, norms, and regularly recurring patterns of social interaction that function as mechanisms of social control; and
- aggregate emergent behaviors or ways of life that distinguish the area from others around it" (Schwirian, 1983, p. 84)
These ideas set the stage for an entire generation of researchers focused on discovering the latent spatial structure in social relations. What is particularly important about the framework Schwirian articulates is encapsulated in the last two bullets. The Chicago School and its devotees maintained a focus on human behavior, cultural norms, and assimilation, which they viewed as having a reflexive relationship with residential arrangements. Early approaches were therefore driven by a focus on identifying, isolating, and quantifying the social processes that led to territorialism, “defended communities”, and segregation, among other emergent behaviors (Suttles, 1972).
One of the earliest innovations in neighborhood empirical work was the notion that social processes, which are difficult to observe, could be treated as latent variables and modeled using easily obtained Census data, similar to the ways that psychologists were beginning to model unobservable personality traits in individuals. The first studies deploying this technique were known as Social Area Analyses (SAA), and were developed to help understand the shifting patterns of segregation and urbanization that began in the 1950s. Although social area analysis has been long studied and its lineage is well-known, it is important to remember that its early emphasis on natural science and social psychology generated a empirical search for the fundamental axes of community differentiation–the laws of social physics that described how segregation and city living restructured the life course.
Social Area Analysis
Social area analysis was first devised by Shevky & Williams (1949) and uses factor analysis to isolate and measure what the authors conceived as three essential dimensions of urban spatial structure:
- urbanization - measured by manifest variables fertility, women in the labor force, and single-family dwelling units
- social rank - measured by manifest variables occupation, educational attainment, and rent
- segregation - measured by an “index of isolation for selected ethnic and foreign-born groups” (Bell, 1953)
Together, Shevky and Bell postulated, these three constructs accounted for the majority of the differences between population groups living in the city. The Shevky-Bell hypothesis, as it is now known, holds that urbanization, segregation, and social rank are the defining forces that structure urban life and influence a variety of behaviors like household formation and participation in formal organizations (Bell, 1953; Spielman & Thill, 2008). Implicit in SAA is that these factors have theoretical connections to behavior. Urbanization, for example may lead to declining birth rates as women stop bearing children and join the workforce, and segregation may lead to predictable residential patterns as immigrants and ethnic minorities form enclaves for mutual benefit[^1].
Following their identification, the latent factors are used as input to cluster analysis, used to group neighborhoods into similar types. Describing Shevky’s original conceptual framework for SAA, Herbert (1967, p. 42) articulates a case that modern urban industrialism is characterized by unavoidable “changes in the distribution of skills, changes in the organisation of productive activity, and changes in the composition of population. Associated with these three main trends are the expressions of social differentiation which become more marked over time.
Thus, Shevky & Williams’s SAA specifies three goals: first, SAA specifies a quantitative framework for capturing these three essential dimensions of social transformation; second, SAA finds groups of spatial units (neighborhoods, in theory) that are similar along each of the three dimensions. By grouping the neighborhoods into categories, Shevky hoped to capture nonlinear dynamics that might result from the interaction of the three components. Third, SAA uses the resulting "social area" categories as lenses and explanatory variables for other urban inquiries (Brindley & Raine, 1979).
Shevky and Williams’s initial work focused on Los Angeles, and soon after it was published, Bell (1953) reimplemented SAA in San Francisco, using his results first to examine the generalizability of the original L.A. study, (Bell1955?) and later to study spatially stratified participation in organizations, and informal social relations in different neighborhood types (Bell & Boat, 1957; Bell & Force, 1956). A number of replications were also performed to test the stability of the Shevky-Bell hypothesis, and whether the same general structure appeared in other American cities, which it often did (Arsdol et al., 1958b; Arsdol et al., 1958a; Greer, 1956; Schmid, 1950; Schmid et al., 1958).
Despites some converging results from different cities, the replication studies often were contentious. Some objected to the use of SAA, arguing that it lacked foundations in social theory and, apart from interesting patterns, provided little insight into the causes and consequences of urban ills (Hawley & Duncan, 1957; Van Arsdol et al., 1961, 1962). In a particularly poignant critique, Hawley & Duncan (1957) question whether urbanization, segregation, and social rank are the defining characteristics of cities, and whether the social areas analysis of these variables provides valuable insight into urban life. Put bluntly, they argue that the ‘social area’ lacks scientific rigor because it “has provided no theory that explains why areas tend to be homogeneous or otherwise, or that predicts the degree of homogeneity to be observed” [ p.339].
Apart from methodological issues, some replications called into question whether the three factor structure was a sufficiently robust model to describe American cities. While there was general support for the SAA model, some results were mixed, particularly with respect to the strength and orthogonality of the three factors, leading Anderson & Bean (1961) to question whether the same factor solution would emerge using alternative variables and whether the factors would remain static in number and interpretation.
Anderson & Egeland (1961) probe the question in more depth, finding support for Burgess’s concentric zones theory in terms of urbanization but not with respect to social rank, and Udry (1964) argues that the factor solution is sensitive to the size of the spatial units. In the ongoing debate, even Bell & Greer (1962) conceded that while “there is clearly emergent and presumptive evidence of verified theoretical structure in the Shevky schema of urban analysis, additional specifications, elaboration, and formulation are necessary,” and as more human ecologists heeded his call, exploratory investigations of the factor structure of urban areas blossomed into their own subfield called “factorial ecology.”
Factorial Ecology
Through the 1970s a staggering number of Factorial Ecology (FE) studies appeared in the literature. Unlike its predecessor SAA, which tried to derive three theoretically meaningful constructs using factor analysis, then performed a cluster analysis on those axes to understand urban segmentation, FE is typically more open-ended. Instead, FE is an inductive approach that leverages exploratory factor analysis and eschews clustering. Similar to Anderson & Bean (1961), factorial ecology researchers are interested in how the urban social structure might be modeled if a more diverse set of variables were factored. Following, in FE many social variables are provided to a factor model and components emerge from the data. By examining which variables load strongly on which factors, researchers can intuit the conceptual interpretation of each component.
Conceptually, factorial ecology borrows from psychological personality research and psychometrics, modifying “factors of the mind” into factors of the neighborhood (Palm & Caruso, 1972). Berry (1971) and Rees (1971) develop “factor models” that borrow from psychological personality research and factor labeling in factorial ecology. Factor models are sensitive to the choice of rotation (oblique or orthogonal) and the estimation procedure used (Hunter, 1972).
Accordingly, many of the results that have emerged from studies of factorial ecology should be treated with skepticism until it can be shown that they are robust to the choice of factor model used (Newton & Johnston, 1976; Perle, 1979; Taylor & Parkes, 1975; Salins1971?). Others have criticized factorial ecology for lacking theory, arguing that it amounts to quantitative fishing, since any derived factor structure can be explained ex-post-facto.
Despite these criticisms, FE studies have been undertaken all over the world and its diverse applications were even featured in a special issue of Economic Geography (Berry, 1971). Scholars have canvassed a wide variety of continents, cultures, and class-systems, including Ireland (Parker, 1975) Sweden (Janson, 1971), the Middle East (Landay, 1971), India (Berry, 1971; Rees1969?), Canada (Bourne & Barber, 1971; Murdie, 1969), and Brazil (Morris1971?), in addition to Los Angeles, Chicago (Hunter, 1971), a wide variety of other American cities (Palm & Caruso, 1972), and more.
Despite this broad applicability–or perhaps because of it–Landay (1971) raises the issue of generalizability and cultural sensitivity when applying factorial ecology in different contexts. Of particular interest is the method by which variables are chosen. If the focus is on a “contextual mode of inquiry,” how much hyperlocal context needs to be embedded in the data, and how “standardized” might be the results? Perfect contextual data is infinitely nuanced by definition, and any attempt to distill a set of “standardized” results is, therefore, off the mark. Thus “if the goal is to make broad descriptive statements, factor analysis may be the appropriate technique, but if the goal is to make statements concerning relationships among specific variables of theoretical interest, correlation and regression methods would appear to be more appropriate” (Berry, 1971, p. 214)
Ecometrics
Following the initial excitement in factorial ecology during the 1960s and 1970s, the practice quickly fell out of vogue and lay mostly dormant through the 1980s and 1990s, presumably in part due to its inability to address methodological critiques. After this lull, however, explorations into the factor structure of communities were revived by Raudenbush & Sampson (1999) in a seminal article introducing a newly minted sub-field they term “Ecometrics.”
To overcome the problems of factorial ecology in the earlier generation, Raudenbush & Sampson propose several improvements to the methodology. First, they argue that while large scale databases like the US Census contain a variety of useful data, they typically fail to capture many of the most important ecological properties of neighborhoods. Instead, Raudenbush & Sampson advocate the use of item-response models tailored specifically to collect information about community structures.
In conjunction with the proposed data collection devices, they encourage the use of confirmatory factor analysis (CFA), as opposed to the exploratory factor analyses (EFA) employed by factor ecologists. Confirmatory factor analysis is a special case of structural equation modeling which, unlike its exploratory cousin, allows social scientists to test a-priori theories about factor relationships by specifying a measurement model that describes how particular variables should load onto designated theoretical latent constructs; in so doing, CFA provides an inferential framework for testing whether the social construct under consideration is supported by the data.
In this way ecometrics is a marked departure from factor ecology (and arguably a return to Chicago School ideals); whereas the latter is concerned with exploratory urban research, using diverse datasets to examine emergent factors and developing post-hoc interpretations of them, the former is concerned with deductive research. A theory about why certain variables are presumed to load into semantically-meaningful factors is stated formally, arguments justifying this are made, and then statistical tests of fit are performed to interrogate these claims (Mujahid et al., 2007).
Ecometrics is still a fledgling methodology, but it has already been shown capable of developing valid and reliable measures of social constructs like collective efficacy, physical disorder, and social disorder, which have important implications for human behavior (Raudenbush & Sampson, 1999; Sampson & Raudenbush, 1999; Sampson2002b?; sampson2012great?). The predominant barrier to adoption in ecometric research has been the costly requirement of systematic social observation (Sampson & Raudenbush, 1999).
Part of the push for ecometrics was that large-scale administrative data (e.g. the Census) often lack information about the most important ecological properties of communities, and thus novel (and expensive) data collection strategies are necessary. Recently, however, researchers have attempted to make ecometric research more accessible by incorporating “big data” sources and “virtual audits” (O’Brien et al., 2015; Sampson, 2017; Bader2015a?; Bader2017?). It seems likely that ecometric analysis will continue to grow in popularity, particularly as additional datasets and new applications materialize.
Meanwhile, however, ecometric studies are relatively rare, and the more common practice, by far, is the development of neighborhood classifications and typologies. Instead of factor analysis, these studies employ cluster analysis to identify groups of neighborhoods (i.e. census tracts) whose racial, economic, physical and other attributes are internally homogeneous. This approach is discussed in detail in the following section.
factor analysis and latent constructs
“Unfortunately, many of the theoretical distinctions between factor analysis and components analysis are lost when one of the major commercial packages claims to be doing factor analysis by doing principal components. Basic concepts of reliability and structural equation modeling are built upon the logic of factor analysis and most discussions of theoretical constructs are discussions of factors rather than components. There is a reason for this. The pragmatic similarity between the three sets of models should not be used as an excuse for theoretical sloppiness.”
Barrett (2007), Finch (2020), Gore & Widiger (2013), Hengartner et al. (2014), Irwing (2018), Lawley & Maxwell (1962), Meredith & Teresi (2006), O’Connor (2002), Osborne (2019), Revelle & Wilt (2013), Revelle (n.d.), Velicer & Jackson (1990)
factorial ecology (unsupervised/EFA)
Berry (1971), Hunter (1972), Murdie (1969), Newton & Johnston (1976), Palm & Caruso (1972), Parker (1975), Perle (1979), Berry & Rees (1969), Rees (1971), Taylor & Parkes (1975), Timms (1970)
Clark et al. (1974), Johnston et al. (2004), Newton & Johnston (1976), Silber (1989), Steiger (2009), Wirth & Edwards (2007)
Abu-Lughod (1969), Bell (1954), Berry & Spodek (1971), Bourne & Barber (1971), Golden & Earp (2012), Goldstein (2018), Hawley (1950), Landay (1971), Park (1952), Perle (1979), Perle (1983), Schmid (1950), Schmid et al. (1958)
ecometrics (supervised/CFA)
Mujahid et al. (2007), O’Brien & Wilson (2011), O’Brien & Kauffman (2013), O’Brien et al. (2015), Raudenbush & Sampson (1999), Sampson (2017)
Hipp (2010), Kuipers et al. (2012), de Leon et al. (2009), Ross & Mirowsky (2001), Sampson & Raudenbush (1999), Sampson & Raudenbush (2004), Sampson et al. (2019), Stockdale et al. (2007)
Software
- geopandas
- scikit-learn
- factor_analyzer
- geosnap
:::
Abu-Lughod, J. L. (1969). Testing the
Theory of
Social Area Analysis:
The Ecology of
Cairo,
Egypt.
American Sociological Review,
34(2), 198.
https://doi.org/10.2307/2092177
Anderson, T. R., & Bean, L. L. (1961). The
Shevky-Bell Social Areas:
Confirmation of
Results and a
Reinterpretation.
Social Forces,
40(2), 119–124.
https://doi.org/10.2307/2574289
Anderson, T. R., & Egeland, J. A. (1961). Spatial
Aspects of
Social Area Analysis.
American Sociological Review,
26(3), 392.
https://doi.org/10.2307/2090666
Arsdol, M. D. V., Camilleri, S. F., & Schmid, C. F. (1958a). The
Generality of
Urban Social Area Indexes.
American Sociological Review,
23(3), 277.
https://doi.org/10.2307/2089241
Arsdol, M. D., Camilleri, S. F., & Schmid, C. F. (1958b). An
Application of the
Shevky Social Area Indexes to a
Model of
Urban Society.
Social Forces,
37(1), 26–32.
https://doi.org/10.2307/2573775
Barrett, P. (2007). Structural equation modelling:
Adjudging model fit.
Personality and Individual Differences,
42(5), 815–824.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2006.09.018
Bell, W. (1953). The
Social Areas of the
San Francisco Bay Region.
American Sociological Review,
18(1), 39.
https://doi.org/10.2307/2087847
Bell, W. (1954). A
Probability Model for the
Measurement of
Ecological Segregation.
Social Forces,
32(4), 357–364.
https://doi.org/10.2307/2574118
Bell, W., & Boat. (1957). Urban
Neighborhoods and
Informal Social Relations.
American Journal of Sociology,
62(4), 391–398.
https://doi.org/10.2307/2772880
Bell, W., & Force, M. T. (1956). Urban
Neighborhood Types and
Participation in
Formal Associations.
American Sociological Review,
21(1), 25.
https://doi.org/10.2307/2089336
Bell, W., & Greer, S. (1962). Social
Area Analysis and
Its Critics.
The Pacific Sociological Review,
5(1), 3–9.
https://doi.org/10.2307/1388270
Berry, B. J. L. (1971). Introduction:
The Logic and
Limitations of
Comparative Factorial Ecology.
Economic Geography,
47(4), 209.
https://doi.org/10.2307/143204
Berry, B. J. L., & Rees, P. H. (1969). The
Factorial Ecology of
Calcutta.
American Journal of Sociology,
74(5), 445–491.
https://doi.org/10.1086/224681
Berry, B. J. L., & Spodek, H. (1971). Comparative
Ecologies of
Large Indian Cities.
Economic Geography,
47(1), 266.
https://doi.org/10.2307/143209
Bourne, L. S., & Barber, G. M. (1971). Ecological
Patterns of
Small Urban Centers in
Canada.
Economic Geography,
47(1), 258.
https://doi.org/10.2307/143208
Brindley, T. S., & Raine, J. W. (1979). Social
Area Analysis and
Planning Research.
Urban Studies,
16(3), 273–289.
https://doi.org/10.1080/713702552
Clark, D., Davies, W. K. D., & Johnston, R. J. (1974). The
Application of
Factor Analysis in
Human Geography.
The Statistician,
23(3/4), 259.
https://doi.org/10.2307/2987583
de Leon, C. F. M., Cagney, K. A., Bienias, J. L., Barnes, L. L., Skarupski, K. A., Scherr, P. A., & Evans, D. A. (2009). Neighborhood
Social Cohesion and
Disorder in
Relation To Walking in
Community-Dwelling Older Adults.
Journal of Aging and Health,
21(1), 155–171.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0898264308328650
Finch, W. H. (2020). Using
Fit Statistic Differences to
Determine the
Optimal Number of
Factors to
Retain in an
Exploratory Factor Analysis.
Educational and Psychological Measurement,
80(2), 217–241.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164419865769
Golden, S. D., & Earp, J. A. L. (2012). Social
Ecological Approaches to
Individuals and
Their Contexts:
Twenty Years of
Health Education &
Behavior Health Promotion Interventions.
Health Education and Behavior,
39(3), 364–372.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1090198111418634
Goldstein, A. (2018). The
Social Ecology of
Speculation:
Community Organization and
Non-occupancy Investment in the
U.
S.
Housing Bubble.
American Sociological Review, 000312241880821.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0003122418808212
Gore, W. L., & Widiger, T. A. (2013). The
DSM-5 dimensional trait model and five-factor models of general personality.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
122(3), 816–821.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032822
Greer, S. (1956). Urbanism
Reconsidered:
A Comparative Study of
Local Areas in a
Metropolis.
American Sociological Review,
21(1), 19.
https://doi.org/10.2307/2089335
Hawley, A. H. (1950).
Human Ecology: A Theory of Community Structure. Ronald Press.
https://books.google.com/books?id=_GvXAQAACAAJ
Hawley, A. H., & Duncan, O. D. (1957). Social
Area Analysis:
A Critical Appraisal.
Land Economics,
33(4), 337.
https://doi.org/10.2307/3144311
Hengartner, M. P., Ajdacic-Gross, V., Rodgers, S., Müller, M., & Rössler, W. (2014). The joint structure of normal and pathological personality:
Further evidence for a dimensional model.
Comprehensive Psychiatry,
55(3), 667–674.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2013.10.011
Herbert, D. T. (1967). Social
Area Analysis: A
British Study.
Urban Studies,
4(1), 41–60.
https://doi.org/10.1080/00420986720080031
Hipp, J. R. (2010). Micro-structure in micro-neighborhoods:
A new social distance measure, and its effect on individual and aggregated perceptions of crime and disorder.
Social Networks,
32(2), 148–159.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2009.11.001
Hunter, A. (1971). The
Ecology of
Chicago:
Persistence and
Change, 1930-1960.
American Journal of Sociology,
77(3), 425–444.
https://doi.org/10.2307/2776178
Hunter, A. (1975). The
Loss of
Community:
An Empirical Test Through Replication.
American Sociological Review,
40(5), 537.
https://doi.org/10.2307/2094194
Hunter, A. A. (1972). Factorial
Ecology:
A Critique and
Some Suggestions.
Demography,
9(1), 107.
https://doi.org/10.2307/2060548
Irwing, F. P. (Ed.). (2018). The Wiley handbook of psychometric testing: A multidisciplinary reference on survey, scale, and test development (First Edition). Wiley Blackwell.
Janson, C. (1971). A
Preliminary Report on
Swedish Urban Spatial Structure.
Economic Geography,
47, 249.
https://doi.org/10.2307/143207
Johnston, R., Jones, K., Burgess, S. M., Propper, C., Sarker, R., & Bolster, A. (2004). Scale,
Factor Analyses, and
Neighborhood Effects.
Geographical Analysis,
36(4), 350–368.
https://doi.org/10.1353/geo.2004.0016
Kuipers, M. A. G., van Poppel, M. N. M., van den Brink, W., Wingen, M., & Kunst, A. E. (2012). The association between neighborhood disorder, social cohesion and hazardous alcohol use:
A national multilevel study.
Drug and Alcohol Dependence,
126(1-2), 27–34.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2012.04.008
Landay, S. (1971). The
Ecology of
Islamic Cities:
The Case for the
Ethnocity.
Economic Geography,
47(1), 303.
https://doi.org/10.2307/143212
Lawley, D. N., & Maxwell, A. E. (1962). Factor
Analysis as a
Statistical Method.
Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series D (The Statistician),
12(3), 209–229.
https://doi.org/10.2307/2986915
Meredith, W., & Teresi, J. A. (2006). An
Essay on
Measurement and
Factorial Invariance.
Medical Care,
44(11), S69.
https://doi.org/10.1097/01.mlr.0000245438.73837.89
Mujahid, M. S., Diez Roux, A. V., Morenoff, J. D., & Raghunathan, T. (2007). Assessing the
Measurement Properties of
Neighborhood Scales:
From Psychometrics to
Ecometrics.
American Journal of Epidemiology,
165(8), 858–867.
https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwm040
Murdie, R. A. (1969).
Factorial ecology of metropolitan Toronto, 1951-1961: An essay on the social geography of the city. [University of Chicago] Dept. of Geography.
https://books.google.com/books?id=JjwDAAAAMAAJ
Newton, P. W., & Johnston, R. J. (1976). Residential
Area Characteristics and
Residential Area Homogeneity:
Further Thoughts on
Extensions to the
Factorial Ecology Method.
Environment and Planning A: Economy and Space,
8(5), 543–552.
https://doi.org/10.1068/a080543
O’Brien, D. T., & Kauffman, R. A. (2013). Broken
Windows and
Low Adolescent Prosociality:
Not Cause and
Consequence, but
Co-symptoms of
Low Collective Efficacy.
American Journal of Community Psychology,
51(3-4), 359–369.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10464-012-9555-1
O’Brien, D. T., Sampson, R. J., & Winship, C. (2015). Ecometrics in the
Age of
Big Data.
Sociological Methodology,
45(1), 101–147.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0081175015576601
O’Connor, B. P. (2002). A
Quantitative Review of the
Comprehensiveness of the
Five-Factor Model in
Relation to
Popular Personality Inventories.
Assessment,
9(2), 188–203.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191102092010
Osborne, J. W. (2019).
What is Rotating in Exploratory Factor Analysis? Practical Assessment, Research, and Evaluation,
20(1).
https://doi.org/10.7275/hb2g-m060
Palm, R., & Caruso, D. (1972). Factor
Labelling In Factorial Ecology.
Annals of the Association of American Geographers,
62(1), 122–133.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8306.1972.tb00848.x
Park, R. E. (1952). Human communities; the city and human ecology. In
Collected papers, v. 2. https://doi.org/10.2307/2087814
Parker, J. B. A. J. (1975). The Factorial Ecology of Dublin: A Preliminary Investigation. Economic and Social Review.
Perle, E. D. (1979). Variable
Mix and
Factor Stability in
Urban Ecology.
Geographical Analysis,
11(4), 410–414.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1538-4632.1979.tb00708.x
Perle, E. D. (1983). Ecology of urban social change—
An American example.
Urban Ecology,
7(4), 307–324.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0304-4009(83)90018-9
Raudenbush, S. W., & Sampson, R. J. (1999). Ecometrics:
Toward a
Science of
Assessing Ecological Settings, with
Application to the
Systematic Social Observation of
Neighborhoods.
Sociological Methodology,
29(1), 1–41.
https://doi.org/10.1111/0081-1750.00059
Rees, P. H. (1971). Factorial
Ecology:
An Extended Definition,
Survey, and
Critique of the
Field.
Economic Geography,
47(4), 220.
https://doi.org/10.2307/143205
Revelle, W. (n.d.).
The Personality Project: An introduction to psychometric theory. Retrieved December 19, 2023, from
https://personality-project.org/r/book/
Revelle, W., & Wilt, J. (2013). The
General Factor of
Personality:
A General Critique.
Journal of Research in Personality,
47(5), 493–504.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2013.04.012
Ross, C. E., & Mirowsky, J. (2001). Neighborhood
Disadvantage,
Disorder, and
Health.
Journal of Health and Social Behavior,
42(3), 258.
https://doi.org/10.2307/3090214
Sampson, L., Martins, S. S., Yu, S., Chiavegatto Filho, A. D. P., Andrade, L. H., Viana, M. C., Medina-Mora, M. E., Benjet, C., Torres, Y., Piazza, M., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Cia, A. H., Stagnaro, J. C., Zaslavsky, A. M., Kessler, R. C., & Galea, S. (2019). The relationship between neighborhood-level socioeconomic characteristics and individual mental disorders in five cities in
Latin America: Multilevel models from the
World Mental Health Surveys.
Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology,
54(2), 157–170.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-018-1595-x
Sampson, R. J. (2017). Urban sustainability in an age of enduring inequalities:
Advancing theory and ecometrics for the 21st-century city.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,
114(34), 8957–8962.
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1614433114
Sampson, R. J., & Raudenbush, S. W. (1999). Systematic
Social Observation of
Public Spaces:
A New Look at
Disorder in
Urban Neighborhoods.
American Journal of Sociology,
105(3), 603–651.
https://doi.org/10.1086/210356
Sampson, R. J., & Raudenbush, S. W. (2004). Seeing
Disorder:
Neighborhood Stigma and the
Social Construction of
“Broken Windows.” Social Psychology Quarterly,
67(4), 319–342.
https://doi.org/10.1177/019027250406700401
Schmid, C. F. (1950).
GENERALIZATIONS CONCERNING THE ECOLOGY OF THE AMERICAN CITY.
American Sociological Review,
15(2), 264–281.
https://doi.org/10.2307/2086791
Schmid, C. F., MacCannell, E. H., & van Arsdol Jr., M. D. (1958).
THE ECOLOGY OF THE AMERICAN CITY:
FURTHER COMPARISON AND VALIDATION OF GENERALIZATIONS.
American Sociological Review,
23(4), 392–401.
https://doi.org/10.2307/2088802
Schwirian, K. P. (1983). Models of
Neighborhood Change.
Annual Review of Sociology,
9(1), 83–102.
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.so.09.080183.000503
Shevky, E., & Williams, M. (1949).
The social areas of Los Angeles: Analysis and typology.
Published for the John Randolph Haynes and Dora Haynes Foundation by the University of California Press.
https://books.google.com/books?id=nrUPAQAAMAAJ
Silber, J. (1989). Factor components, population subgroups and the computation of the {}
Gini{} index of inequality.
The Review of Economics and Statistics,
71, 107–115.
https://doi.org/10.2307/1928057
Spielman, S. E., & Thill, J.-C. (2008). Social area analysis, data mining, and
GIS.
Computers, Environment and Urban Systems,
32(2), 110–122.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compenvurbsys.2007.11.004
Steiger, J. H. (2009). Exploratory Factor Analysis with R (p. 10).
Stockdale, S. E., Wells, K. B., Tang, L., Belin, T. R., Zhang, L., & Sherbourne, C. D. (2007). The
Importance of
Social Context:
Neighborhood Stressors,
Stress-Buffering Mechanisms, and
Alcohol,
Drug, and
Mental Health Disorders.
Soc Sci Med,
65(9), 1867–1881.
https://doi.org/10.1242/jcs.03292.Multiple
Suttles, G. D. (1972).
The Social Construction of Communities. University of Chicago Press.
https://books.google.com/books?id=IsqcQgAACAAJ
Taylor, P. J., & Parkes, D. N. (1975). A
Kantian View of the
City:
A Factorial-Ecology Experiment in
Space and
Time.
Environment and Planning A: Economy and Space,
7(6), 671–688.
https://doi.org/10.1068/a070671
Timms, D. W. G. (1970). Comparative factorial ecology: Some
New Zealand examples.
Environment and Planning,
2, 455–467.
https://doi.org/10.1068/a020455
Udry, J. R. (1964). Increasing
Scale and
Spatial Differentiation:
New Tests of
Two Theories from
Shevky and
Bell.
Social Forces,
42(4), 403–413.
https://doi.org/10.2307/2574983
Van Arsdol, M. D., Camilleri, S. F., & Schmid, C. F. (1961). An
Investigation of the
Utility of
Urban Typology.
The Pacific Sociological Review,
4(1), 26–32.
https://doi.org/10.2307/1388484
Van Arsdol, M. D., Camilleri, S. F., & Schmid, C. F. (1962). Further
Comments on the
Utility of
Urban Typology.
The Pacific Sociological Review,
5(1), 9–13.
https://doi.org/10.2307/1388271
Velicer, W. F., & Jackson, D. N. (1990). Component
Analysis versus
Common Factor Analysis:
Some issues in
Selecting an
Appropriate Procedure.
Multivariate Behavioral Research,
25(1), 1–28.
https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327906mbr2501_1
Wirth, R. J., & Edwards, M. C. (2007). Item factor analysis:
Current approaches and future directions.
Psychological Methods,
12(1), 58–79.
https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.12.1.58